Search This Blog
Get ready to be enthralled, enlightened, and engaged as we curate a tapestry of fascinating information for you to discover and share with the globe.
Featured
- Get link
- X
- Other Apps
Should You Try to Pay off Sleep Debt on Weekends?
The short version
- A recent study explored whether trying to catch up on sleep on the weekend can counter the negative effects of workday sleep loss on metabolic health.
- Weekend recovery sleep wasn’t enough to repay the workday sleep debt and led to similar weight gain to when sleep loss continued over the weekend.
- In some ways, weekend recovery sleep resulted in worse outcomes than continued sleep loss. For example, weekend recovery sleep led to greater deterioration in insulin sensitivity, a key determinant of risk of developing diabetes.
- Some of the negative effects of sleep loss seemed to be more prominent in men.
- These findings don’t necessarily mean that you shouldn’t try to catch up on lost sleep when you can.
- After catching up on lost sleep, avoid consuming caffeine and/or other stimulants, spend at least 30 minutes outdoors in daylight as early as possible after waking, and stick to your usual pre-sleep routine.
The full version
We all occasionally have periods of time in which we don’t get as much sleep as we’d like. When we repeatedly restrict our sleep like this, we soon suffer from widespread deterioration in metabolic health, brain function, and more.
As I’ve discussed on the blog before, when people who have been habitually restricting their sleep then extend their sleep, they experience improved health and performance. These sleep extension studies generally involve prolonging sleep for at least a week, but a more common scenario is for people to try to pay off sleep debt on weekends after restricting their sleep before workdays.
Scientists have therefore attempted to study the effects of this pattern. The most recent of these experiments was done by Ken Wright from the University of Colorado Boulder, a scientist whose work I admire greatly. The study explored the effects of weekend recovery sleep on food intake and insulin sensitivity, a key determinant of risk of developing diabetes. The research received lots of attention and sparked much discussion, so in this blog we’ll explore it in more detail.
The experiment was impressively well carried out, and it must have been tough to do. It isn’t the easiest study to describe and make sense of, and this is perhaps why some of the more simplistic descriptions of it in the popular press are a bit misleading. I’ll do my best to not get bogged down in minutiae, but I’m keen to share details of the research that are important to interpreting it.
To begin, let’s review what the scientists did.
Study design
The researchers first established participants’ baseline needs
The participants were healthy but sedentary young adults (50% female). To document their baseline sleep patterns, they wore Fitbit-like devices for one week before visiting the lab. For the last three of these days, they were given baseline diets designed to maintain their bodyweights, and participants were prescribed brief exercise sessions to standardize their physical activity at this time too.
Participants were divided into three groups: control, sleep restriction, and weekend recovery sleep
Participants were then divided into three groups, and all of these people then spent nine nights in the lab:
- Control: nine hours in bed each night (bedtime as established at baseline).
- Sleep restriction: five hours in bed each night (they went to bed two hours earlier than at baseline and woke up two hours earlier than at baseline).
- Weekend “recovery”:
-
- From Monday night to Thursday night, participants had five hours in bed each night (they went to bed two hours later than at baseline and woke up two hours earlier than at baseline).
- On Friday night, participants went to bed two hours earlier than at baseline but were allowed to sleep in on Saturday morning and nap during Saturday daytime.
- On Saturday night, participants chose their bedtimes and were allowed to sleep in on Sunday morning and nap during Sunday daytime.
- On Sunday night, participants chose their bedtimes but then had to wake up two hours earlier than at baseline.
- The following Monday night and Tuesday night, participants had five hours in bed each night (they went to bed two hours later than at baseline and woke up two hours earlier than at baseline).
During the nine nights, the participants had access to as much food as they wished. They were kept indoors in typical room lighting (the rooms had windows), and they were sedentary other than two, 20-minute exercise sessions each day in which they repeatedly stepped on and off low steps.
The scientists measured the timing of participants’ bodies clocks and their sleep
On the final day before the experiment, participants were kept in very dim light to assess their melatonin rhythms. This was repeated on the first Tuesday and the second Monday. As I explain in the humanOS courses on our bodies’ clocks (available to Pro members), melatonin is primarily synthesized during the biological night and is the best marker of the timing of the clocks in our brains that orchestrate when our bodies are prepared for certain activities (physical activity, digesting food, and so on).
To record sleep during the experiment, the researchers used polysomnography on the first Monday and Tuesday, the Friday, the Saturday, the Sunday, and the second Monday. Polysomnography basically entails covering people in electrodes and wires to record brainwaves, blood oxygen concentration, heart rate, breathing, and eye and leg movements. It’s uncomfortable, but it’s the gold-standard way of measuring sleep.
The scientists measured participants’ insulin sensitivity
To determine the effects of sleep patterns on how effectively the participants disposed of glucose in their blood, the scientists measured intravenous fasted insulin sensitivity at baseline (on the first Monday) and on the morning of the second Tuesday.
Now that I’ve spent so long describing what the scientists did, what did they find?
Study results
Weekend recovery sleep wasn’t enough to pay off the workday sleep debt, and recovery sleep led to delayed melatonin synthesis
Sleep restriction led to more than 12 hours of lost sleep over the whole working week. Needless to say, weekend recovery sleep didn’t allow participants to fully repay this debt – from Friday night to Monday morning, they only slept a total of 1.1 hours more than they did at baseline. The analysis of the polysomnography data collected on the weekend also suggests that participants still had a sleep debt after weekend recovery sleep.
On the second Monday, the melatonin rhythms of participants in the sleep restriction group were delayed by about 25 minutes. However, after weekend recovery sleep, participants’ melatonin rhythms were delayed more still – by roughly 1.4 hours. This disparity between groups probably resulted from differences in their patterns of light exposure.
Despite reducing energy intake late in the day, weekend recovery sleep didn’t stop participants from gaining weight
During sleep restriction, participants consumed substantially more calories than their baseline needs, and much of this extra energy came after dinner. As I have discussed before on the blog and elsewhere, consuming food late in the day tends to be more obesogenic than consuming it earlier.
Weekend recovery sleep reduced energy intake from post-dinner snacks during the weekend, as well as energy intake from pre-dinner snacks during and after the weekend. This suggests some positive effects of recovery sleep on dietary choices.
Nevertheless, by the end of the study, participants’ bodyweights increased similarly in the sleep restriction and weekend recovery groups (by 1.4 and 1.3 kg, respectively). So, the effect of recovery sleep on energy intake didn’t prevent weight gain.
Insulin sensitivity declined most in the weekend recovery sleep condition
In the control group, insulin sensitivity did not change during the study. In the sleep restriction group, however, whole-body insulin sensitivity declined 13% below baseline after eights nights. By this time in the weekend recovery group, whole-body insulin sensitivity was 27% worse than at baseline.
Men might be more negatively affected by weekend recovery sleep
While the study wasn’t really designed to test this, the researchers explored whether men and women differed in their responses during the experiment. During weekend recovery sleep, women did not sleep longer than at baseline, despite spending more time in bed. What’s more, while men in the weekend recovery sleep group consumed substantially more energy during the first work week (72%), the weekend (40%), and the second work week (65%), these changes were smaller in women (30%, 2%, and 25%, respectively). Given these findings, it’s no surprise that bodyweight increased in men (by 3%) but not women.
What should we make of all of this?
In summary, weekend recovery sleep wasn’t enough to repay the workday sleep debt and led to similar weight gain to continuous sleep restriction. In some ways, weekend recovery sleep resulted in worse outcomes than sleep restriction – after all, weekend recovery sleep led to a larger delay in melatonin synthesis and greater deterioration in insulin sensitivity. Believe it or not, I’ve actually glossed over many details of the study in this blog – it had a lot of moving parts and nuances.
Anyway, based on the results, trying to catch up on sleep loss seems a bad idea, right?
I don’t think so. Here are some reasons why:
First, looking at all available studies (including 1, 2, and 3), whether weekend recovery sleep is beneficial or detrimental likely depends on what is being measured, and please note that the latest findings differ from those of similar previous research. For example, one experiment that used similar methods to Wright’s found that weekend recovery sleep reversed the negative effects of workday sleep loss on insulin sensitivity. These results are in line with those of an earlier study of men who habitually restricted their sleep during the working week.
Next, ask yourself this: How representative is the latest study of the kind of workday sleep loss you experience?
There are good data on how people in countries such as the US spend their time. A brilliant recent analysis of how Americans used their time between 2003 and 2016 showed that US citizens have been spending more time in bed at night in recent years.
This makes me happy!
What’s more, among US adults, time in bed on weekdays is typically over 8 hours per night. They do tend to spend longer in bed on weekends, but only a little, and they achieve this through later rise times. Yes, these are of course averages, and they don’t inform us how long these people sleep per se. However, the data do show that the kind of sleep restriction enforced in Wright’s study isn’t necessarily representative of how most of us generally transition between workdays and free days.
Sleep aside, consider also the study setting. The participants had access to as much food as they desired. While not statistically significant, even the control group gained 1 kg, on average. Physical activity was constrained too. To be clear, these variables were consistent between conditions, and the study was very tightly controlled. So, we can attribute differences in outcomes between groups to differences in their sleep. However, if these people were not in what was basically a nine-day long buffet and were permitted to move more, it’s possible that the differences between groups would have differed.
Is it smart to try and repay sleep debt on the weekends? Check out this blog for more...
Click To Tweet
Suggestions
The advice I’ve been giving and will continue to give is to try to catch up on lost sleep when you can. Studies like this certainly make me less sure that this is always the best recommendation, but I do think it’s still smart to try to accumulate as much sleep as you can over time. I think about sleep in the same way that I think about the role energy intake in bodyweight regulation – occasional overeating is fine, the most important thing is calorie intake over longer periods (a week, for instance).
Now, when you sleep in after losing sleep, I suggest you do the following things to avoid your recovery sleep from delaying your body’s clock:
- Consume no stimulants the day after sleeping in (caffeine appears to delay our bodies’ clocks, at least when consumed late in the day).
- Spend at least 30 minutes outside during daylight as early as possible after waking. This will help prevent your body’s clock from drifting later.
- Stick to your usual pre-sleep routine and be sure to keep its timing the same as usual.
Sleep well tonight!
Support
Have you considered becoming a Pro member of humanOS.me? It costs just $9.99 per month, and when you go Pro, you get access to all our courses, tools, recipes, and workouts. Pro members also support our work on blogs and podcasts, so thanks!
The post Should You Try to Pay off Sleep Debt on Weekends? appeared first on humanOS.me.
- Get link
- X
- Other Apps
Popular Posts
5 Productivity Tools For 2017
- Get link
- X
- Other Apps
99 Tools to Enhance Your Freelance Lifestyle
- Get link
- X
- Other Apps
Comments
Post a Comment